DMCA
Numerical Estimation in Children With Autism
Citations
453 |
Autism: Explaining the enigma
- Frith
- 1989
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...autistic children’s difficulties with regards to number. Autism Res 2015, 00: 000–000. VC 2015 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Keywords: autism; mathematics; number; visual perception Introduction Individuals diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition (hereafter, “autism”) are often most well known for their difficulties in social communication. It is now well established, however, that they often show an uneven profile of abilities, including a pattern of typical or often superior performance in other domains, particularly in visuospatial processing [e.g., Frith, 1989; Mottron, Dawson, & Soulieres, 2009; see Simmons et al., 2009, for review]. Number skills are often reported anecdotally and in the mass media as a relative strength for autistic1 people. For example, Sacks [1985] reported the extraordinary behavior of two twins with autism, who could determine instantly the correct numbers of matches dispersed on the floor as 111, and subsequently qualify them as “equal to three times thirty-seven.” This striking ability, which provided the inspiration for the talents of the lead autistic character in the film, Rain Man, was in stark contrast to the twins’ ... |
321 | QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method - Watson, Pelli - 1983 |
274 | Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). The Psychological Corporation: - Wechsler - 1999 |
196 |
Core systems of number
- Feigenson, Dehaene, et al.
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... in arithmetic achievement in autism is striking. Yet as informative as these standardized tests are, they are unable to provide a detailed and comprehensive analysis of why such achievement is so variable. Here, we investigated autistic children’s numerical estimation skills to determine whether individual differences in these skills might be one potential source of the variability in formal arithmetic skills. Researchers typically distinguish between two number systems operating over different numerical ranges, both of which are believed to contribute to children’s mathematical achievement [Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004]. For relatively few items (<4) a “subitizing” system enables a rapid, exact representation of numerosity while an approximate system is utilized to estimate magnitudes when more items are present [see Burr & Ross, 2008]. To our knowledge, only three studies have investigated these numerical estimation skills in autism, both targeting the exact number system. Jarrold and Russell [1997] found that autistic children showed less benefit than comparison children in counting dot stimuli presented in canonical (dots on dice) than noncanonical (distributed randomly) form, and used a less efficient d... |
192 | A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space, and quantity. - Walsh - 2003 |
177 | The extreme male brain theory of autism. - Baron-Cohen - 2002 |
150 | Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. - Mottron, Dawson, et al. - 2006 |
139 | Autism: beyond “theory of mind”, - Frith, Happe - 1994 |
135 | The man who mistook his wife for a hat. - Sacks - 1995 |
128 |
Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature
- Halberda, Mazzocco, et al.
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...roposed that such perceptual operations are generally enhanced in autism, rendering it possible that many autistic individuals might excel at numerosity. Furthermore, it is assumed that the ability to represent numerosity or nonsymbolic numerical magnitude (rather than subitizing) is a prerequisite for the later acquisition of symbolic representations of numerical magnitude [Feigenson et al., 2004]. Consistent with this view, the ability to estimate numerosity correlates strongly with mathematics achievement at different ages [Anobile, Stievano, & Burr, 2013; Gilmore, McCarthy & Spelke, 2010; Halberda, Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008; Piazza et al., 2010; although see Tibber et al., Figure 1. Nonsymbolic estimation: numerosity discrimination task. Children were introduced to two animated characters in a battle to see who can win the most marbles. Children were asked to help the friendly character, Zando (left). They were told they would see two sets of “marbles” briefly and to touch the side of the screen that contains more “marbles.” 2 Aagten-Murphy et al./Numerical estimation in children with autism INSAR 2013]. Individual differences in nonsymbolic estimation (numerosity) could, therefore, provide one explanation for t... |
122 |
Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development
- Siegler, Booth
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ildren to indicate the position of a symbolic digit or a cloud of dots on a “numberline” [Booth & Siegler, 2006]. Younger children tend to overestimate smaller numbers and compress larger numbers at the end of the line, producing estimates that are better fit by a logarithmic than a linear function. Older children and adults tend to produce more accurate, more nearly linear estimates. Importantly, individual differences in children’s symbolic estimation, their ability to map numbers onto space is also linked to variation in mathematical achievement [Booth & Siegler, 2006; Piazza et al., 2010; Siegler & Booth, 2004]. The Present Study The aim of this research was to investigate number estimation in children with autism. First, we assessed cognitively able 8- to 13-year-old children with and without autism on an engaging and developmentally sensitive nonsymbolic estimation (numerosity) task. Second, we determined their symbolic estimation by measuring their ability to map symbolic representations onto space with two numberline tasks. Finally, we also sought to establish the extent to which individual differences in these skills related to differences in mathematical achievement. Method Participants Thirt... |
100 |
Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain.
- Ansari
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nonautistic adolescents, although the groups were not compared statistically [see O’Hearn, Franceroni, Wright, Minshew, & Luna, 2013, for replication and extension]. Although all three studies assess precise enumeration using different methods, none is suggestive of superiorities in the exact system in autism. It remains possible, however, that estimation of approximate number might be enhanced in autism. This skill is typically assessed using psychophysical paradigms whereby participants are briefly shown two patches of dots and asked to report which patch contains more dots [see Fig. 1, and Ansari, 2008, for review]. Some researchers [Mottron, Dawson, Soulieres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006; Plaisted, 2001] have proposed that such perceptual operations are generally enhanced in autism, rendering it possible that many autistic individuals might excel at numerosity. Furthermore, it is assumed that the ability to represent numerosity or nonsymbolic numerical magnitude (rather than subitizing) is a prerequisite for the later acquisition of symbolic representations of numerical magnitude [Feigenson et al., 2004]. Consistent with this view, the ability to estimate numerosity correlates strongly with ma... |
94 | Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation.
- Booth, Siegler
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...lp the friendly character, Zando (left). They were told they would see two sets of “marbles” briefly and to touch the side of the screen that contains more “marbles.” 2 Aagten-Murphy et al./Numerical estimation in children with autism INSAR 2013]. Individual differences in nonsymbolic estimation (numerosity) could, therefore, provide one explanation for the variability in arithmetic abilities in autism. Another potential source of individual differences in mathematics is mapping number to space, asking children to indicate the position of a symbolic digit or a cloud of dots on a “numberline” [Booth & Siegler, 2006]. Younger children tend to overestimate smaller numbers and compress larger numbers at the end of the line, producing estimates that are better fit by a logarithmic than a linear function. Older children and adults tend to produce more accurate, more nearly linear estimates. Importantly, individual differences in children’s symbolic estimation, their ability to map numbers onto space is also linked to variation in mathematical achievement [Booth & Siegler, 2006; Piazza et al., 2010; Siegler & Booth, 2004]. The Present Study The aim of this research was to investigate number estimation in chil... |
78 | Social communication questionnaire. - Rutter, Bailey, et al. - 2003 |
68 | Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. - Holloway, Ansari - 2009 |
53 | Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction. - Gilmore, McCarthy, et al. - 2007 |
53 |
Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia.
- Piazza, Facoetti, et al.
- 2010
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... are generally enhanced in autism, rendering it possible that many autistic individuals might excel at numerosity. Furthermore, it is assumed that the ability to represent numerosity or nonsymbolic numerical magnitude (rather than subitizing) is a prerequisite for the later acquisition of symbolic representations of numerical magnitude [Feigenson et al., 2004]. Consistent with this view, the ability to estimate numerosity correlates strongly with mathematics achievement at different ages [Anobile, Stievano, & Burr, 2013; Gilmore, McCarthy & Spelke, 2010; Halberda, Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008; Piazza et al., 2010; although see Tibber et al., Figure 1. Nonsymbolic estimation: numerosity discrimination task. Children were introduced to two animated characters in a battle to see who can win the most marbles. Children were asked to help the friendly character, Zando (left). They were told they would see two sets of “marbles” briefly and to touch the side of the screen that contains more “marbles.” 2 Aagten-Murphy et al./Numerical estimation in children with autism INSAR 2013]. Individual differences in nonsymbolic estimation (numerosity) could, therefore, provide one explanation for the variability in ari... |
45 |
A visual sense of number
- Burr, Ross
- 2008
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... children’s numerical estimation skills to determine whether individual differences in these skills might be one potential source of the variability in formal arithmetic skills. Researchers typically distinguish between two number systems operating over different numerical ranges, both of which are believed to contribute to children’s mathematical achievement [Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004]. For relatively few items (<4) a “subitizing” system enables a rapid, exact representation of numerosity while an approximate system is utilized to estimate magnitudes when more items are present [see Burr & Ross, 2008]. To our knowledge, only three studies have investigated these numerical estimation skills in autism, both targeting the exact number system. Jarrold and Russell [1997] found that autistic children showed less benefit than comparison children in counting dot stimuli presented in canonical (dots on dice) than noncanonical (distributed randomly) form, and used a less efficient dot-by-dot counting strategy. Gagnon, Mottron, Bherer, and Joanette [2004] showed that, when asked to judge numerosities between 2 and 9 (e.g., “how many squares are on the screen?”), adolescents with autism seemed to sho... |
31 |
Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Plaisted
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...oni, Wright, Minshew, & Luna, 2013, for replication and extension]. Although all three studies assess precise enumeration using different methods, none is suggestive of superiorities in the exact system in autism. It remains possible, however, that estimation of approximate number might be enhanced in autism. This skill is typically assessed using psychophysical paradigms whereby participants are briefly shown two patches of dots and asked to report which patch contains more dots [see Fig. 1, and Ansari, 2008, for review]. Some researchers [Mottron, Dawson, Soulieres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006; Plaisted, 2001] have proposed that such perceptual operations are generally enhanced in autism, rendering it possible that many autistic individuals might excel at numerosity. Furthermore, it is assumed that the ability to represent numerosity or nonsymbolic numerical magnitude (rather than subitizing) is a prerequisite for the later acquisition of symbolic representations of numerical magnitude [Feigenson et al., 2004]. Consistent with this view, the ability to estimate numerosity correlates strongly with mathematics achievement at different ages [Anobile, Stievano, & Burr, 2013; Gilmore, McCarthy & Spelke... |
30 | Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling. - Gilmore, McCarthy, et al. - 2010 |
27 | The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. - Barth, Paladino - 2011 |
24 |
Ability profiles in children with autism: Influence of age and IQ.
- Mayes, Calhoun
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...y Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/aur.1482 VC 2015 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. INSAR Autism Research 00: 00–00, 2015 1 contrary to Baron-Cohen’s [2002] prediction, not all children with autism show strengths in mathematics. Consistent with anecdotal and single-case reports, studies have identified a sizeable proportion of young people with autism [up to 16% of n 5 100; Jones et al., 2009] with exceptional arithmetic processing relative to their intellectual functioning, or “hypercalculia.” Yet between 6% [Jones et al., 2009] and 22% [Mayes & Calhoun, 2003a] of children and adolescents with autism were reported to struggle with calculation and mental arithmetic to an extent that their maths difficulties were incommensurate with intellectual functioning, indicative of a specific disability in arithmetic, or developmental dyscalculia. The amount of variability in arithmetic achievement in autism is striking. Yet as informative as these standardized tests are, they are unable to provide a detailed and comprehensive analysis of why such achievement is so variable. Here, we investigated autistic children’s numerical estimation skills to determine wh... |
24 |
Vision in autism spectrum disorders.
- Simmons, Robertson, et al.
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...r. Autism Res 2015, 00: 000–000. VC 2015 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Keywords: autism; mathematics; number; visual perception Introduction Individuals diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition (hereafter, “autism”) are often most well known for their difficulties in social communication. It is now well established, however, that they often show an uneven profile of abilities, including a pattern of typical or often superior performance in other domains, particularly in visuospatial processing [e.g., Frith, 1989; Mottron, Dawson, & Soulieres, 2009; see Simmons et al., 2009, for review]. Number skills are often reported anecdotally and in the mass media as a relative strength for autistic1 people. For example, Sacks [1985] reported the extraordinary behavior of two twins with autism, who could determine instantly the correct numbers of matches dispersed on the floor as 111, and subsequently qualify them as “equal to three times thirty-seven.” This striking ability, which provided the inspiration for the talents of the lead autistic character in the film, Rain Man, was in stark contrast to the twins’ cognitive ability (IQ<70). This report echoes many reports of s... |
22 |
Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford- Binet:IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism.
- Mayes, Calhoun
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...y Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/aur.1482 VC 2015 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. INSAR Autism Research 00: 00–00, 2015 1 contrary to Baron-Cohen’s [2002] prediction, not all children with autism show strengths in mathematics. Consistent with anecdotal and single-case reports, studies have identified a sizeable proportion of young people with autism [up to 16% of n 5 100; Jones et al., 2009] with exceptional arithmetic processing relative to their intellectual functioning, or “hypercalculia.” Yet between 6% [Jones et al., 2009] and 22% [Mayes & Calhoun, 2003a] of children and adolescents with autism were reported to struggle with calculation and mental arithmetic to an extent that their maths difficulties were incommensurate with intellectual functioning, indicative of a specific disability in arithmetic, or developmental dyscalculia. The amount of variability in arithmetic achievement in autism is striking. Yet as informative as these standardized tests are, they are unable to provide a detailed and comprehensive analysis of why such achievement is so variable. Here, we investigated autistic children’s numerical estimation skills to determine wh... |
20 | Sometimes area counts more than number - Hurewitz, Gelman, et al. - 2006 |
20 | Developmental dyscalculia: prevalence and prognosis - Shalev, Auerbach, et al. - 2000 |
19 | Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. - Smedt, Gilmore - 2011 |
11 | Autism diagnostic observation schedule (WPS edition). - Lord, Rutter, et al. - 1999 |
11 | Enhanced perception in savant syndrome: Patterns, structure and creativity. - Mottron, Dawson, et al. - 2009 |
10 | Reading and arithmetic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Peaks and dips in attainment. - Jones, Happe, et al. - 2009 |
8 |
Is there a link between engineering and autism?
- Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, et al.
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...abilities in autistic savants [e.g., Cowan & Frith, 2009; Soulieres et al., 2010] but whether such superiorities are characteristic of autistic individuals who are not savants is unclear. One popular theoretical account of autism gives us reason to expect that individuals with autism might be generally talented at mathematics. Baron-Cohen’s [2002; Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Aswin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009] theory proposes that males have an inherent drive to understand rule-based systems (i.e., to “systemize”), which he claims is linked to talent in the fields of mathematics and engineering [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997; although see Jarrold & Routh, 1998]. On this account, autistic individuals are considered to have an “extreme form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skills are critical to educational achievement and for achieving independence post full-time education, there are only a handful of studies on this topic [see Chiang & Lin, 2007, for review]. However, with one exception [Iuculano et al., 2014], most st... |
7 |
Mathematical ability of students with Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism: A review of literature.
- Chiang, Lin
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...elds of mathematics and engineering [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997; although see Jarrold & Routh, 1998]. On this account, autistic individuals are considered to have an “extreme form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skills are critical to educational achievement and for achieving independence post full-time education, there are only a handful of studies on this topic [see Chiang & Lin, 2007, for review]. However, with one exception [Iuculano et al., 2014], most studies demonstrate that, 1The term “autistic” is the preferred language of many people on the spectrum (e.g., Sinclair, 1999). In this article, we use this term as well as person-first language to respect the wishes of individuals on the spectrum. From the Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universit€at M€unchen, Munich, Germany (D.A.M.); Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy (D.A.M., D.B.); School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia (D.B., E.P.); Centre ... |
6 | Counting abilities in autism: Possible implications for central coherence theory. - Jarrold, Russell - 1997 |
5 | Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. - Bull, Lee - 2014 |
4 | Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? - Sasanguie, G€obel, et al. - 2013 |
3 | Developmental neuroscience of time and number: implications for autism and other developmental disabilities. - Allman, Pelphrey, et al. - 2012 |
3 |
Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children.
- Anobile, Stievano, et al.
- 2013
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... Dawson, Soulieres, Hubert, & Burack, 2006; Plaisted, 2001] have proposed that such perceptual operations are generally enhanced in autism, rendering it possible that many autistic individuals might excel at numerosity. Furthermore, it is assumed that the ability to represent numerosity or nonsymbolic numerical magnitude (rather than subitizing) is a prerequisite for the later acquisition of symbolic representations of numerical magnitude [Feigenson et al., 2004]. Consistent with this view, the ability to estimate numerosity correlates strongly with mathematics achievement at different ages [Anobile, Stievano, & Burr, 2013; Gilmore, McCarthy & Spelke, 2010; Halberda, Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008; Piazza et al., 2010; although see Tibber et al., Figure 1. Nonsymbolic estimation: numerosity discrimination task. Children were introduced to two animated characters in a battle to see who can win the most marbles. Children were asked to help the friendly character, Zando (left). They were told they would see two sets of “marbles” briefly and to touch the side of the screen that contains more “marbles.” 2 Aagten-Murphy et al./Numerical estimation in children with autism INSAR 2013]. Individual differences in nonsymboli... |
3 |
Do calendrical savants use calculation to answer date questions? A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
- Cowan, Frith
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...y and in the mass media as a relative strength for autistic1 people. For example, Sacks [1985] reported the extraordinary behavior of two twins with autism, who could determine instantly the correct numbers of matches dispersed on the floor as 111, and subsequently qualify them as “equal to three times thirty-seven.” This striking ability, which provided the inspiration for the talents of the lead autistic character in the film, Rain Man, was in stark contrast to the twins’ cognitive ability (IQ<70). This report echoes many reports of superior calculations abilities in autistic savants [e.g., Cowan & Frith, 2009; Soulieres et al., 2010] but whether such superiorities are characteristic of autistic individuals who are not savants is unclear. One popular theoretical account of autism gives us reason to expect that individuals with autism might be generally talented at mathematics. Baron-Cohen’s [2002; Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Aswin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009] theory proposes that males have an inherent drive to understand rule-based systems (i.e., to “systemize”), which he claims is linked to talent in the fields of mathematics and engineering [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997... |
3 |
Is there really a link between engineering and autism? A reply to Baron-Cohen et al., Autism,
- Jarrold, Routh
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... al., 2010] but whether such superiorities are characteristic of autistic individuals who are not savants is unclear. One popular theoretical account of autism gives us reason to expect that individuals with autism might be generally talented at mathematics. Baron-Cohen’s [2002; Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Aswin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009] theory proposes that males have an inherent drive to understand rule-based systems (i.e., to “systemize”), which he claims is linked to talent in the fields of mathematics and engineering [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997; although see Jarrold & Routh, 1998]. On this account, autistic individuals are considered to have an “extreme form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skills are critical to educational achievement and for achieving independence post full-time education, there are only a handful of studies on this topic [see Chiang & Lin, 2007, for review]. However, with one exception [Iuculano et al., 2014], most studies demonstrate that, 1The term “a... |
2 | Are systemizing and autistic traits related to talent and interest in mathematics and engineering? Testing some of the central claims of the empathizingsystemizing theory. - Morsanyi, Primi, et al. - 2012 |
2 | Testing the predictive power of cognitive atypicalities in autism: evidence from a 3-year follow-up study. - Pellicano - 2013 |
2 |
Why I dislike “person first” language.
- Sinclair
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...me form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skills are critical to educational achievement and for achieving independence post full-time education, there are only a handful of studies on this topic [see Chiang & Lin, 2007, for review]. However, with one exception [Iuculano et al., 2014], most studies demonstrate that, 1The term “autistic” is the preferred language of many people on the spectrum (e.g., Sinclair, 1999). In this article, we use this term as well as person-first language to respect the wishes of individuals on the spectrum. From the Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universit€at M€unchen, Munich, Germany (D.A.M.); Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy (D.A.M., D.B.); School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia (D.B., E.P.); Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), UCL Institute of Education, University College London, UK (C.A., N.D., E.K., E.P.) Received July 23, 2014; accepted for publication February 28, 2015 Addr... |
1 | and the Autism Diagnostic Observational Scale–Generic - Questionnaire - 2003 |
1 |
Talent in autism: Hypersystematizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity.
- Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, et al.
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...en.” This striking ability, which provided the inspiration for the talents of the lead autistic character in the film, Rain Man, was in stark contrast to the twins’ cognitive ability (IQ<70). This report echoes many reports of superior calculations abilities in autistic savants [e.g., Cowan & Frith, 2009; Soulieres et al., 2010] but whether such superiorities are characteristic of autistic individuals who are not savants is unclear. One popular theoretical account of autism gives us reason to expect that individuals with autism might be generally talented at mathematics. Baron-Cohen’s [2002; Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Aswin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009] theory proposes that males have an inherent drive to understand rule-based systems (i.e., to “systemize”), which he claims is linked to talent in the fields of mathematics and engineering [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997; although see Jarrold & Routh, 1998]. On this account, autistic individuals are considered to have an “extreme form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skill... |
1 | Quantification judgment in high functioning autism: Superior or different? - Gagnon, Mottron, et al. - 2004 |
1 |
Brain organisation underlying superior mathematical abilities in children with autism.
- Iuculano, Rosenberg-Lee, et al.
- 2014
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... Stott, Bolton, & Goodyer, 1997; although see Jarrold & Routh, 1998]. On this account, autistic individuals are considered to have an “extreme form of the male brain” and, as such, show “hyper-systemizing.” One might, therefore, expect individuals with autism also to show enhanced underlying number and mathematical skills. Despite this widespread belief and the fact that such skills are critical to educational achievement and for achieving independence post full-time education, there are only a handful of studies on this topic [see Chiang & Lin, 2007, for review]. However, with one exception [Iuculano et al., 2014], most studies demonstrate that, 1The term “autistic” is the preferred language of many people on the spectrum (e.g., Sinclair, 1999). In this article, we use this term as well as person-first language to respect the wishes of individuals on the spectrum. From the Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universit€at M€unchen, Munich, Germany (D.A.M.); Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy (D.A.M., D.B.); School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia (D.B., E.P.); Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), UCL Institute of Edu... |
1 | The development of individuation in autism. - O’Hearn, Franceroni, et al. - 2013 |
1 | Wechsler Objective Numerical Dimensions (WOND). - Rust - 1996 |
1 | from http://www.cafemom. com/journals/read/436505/ Soulieres, - Hubert, Rouleauc, et al. - 2013 |
1 | Sensitivity to numoersity is not a unique visuospatial psychophysical predictor of mathematical ability. - Tibber, Manasseh, et al. - 2013 |