DMCA
THE ROLE OF DOMAIN GENERAL COGNITIVE MECHANISMS IN BILINGUAL LANGUAGE PRODUCTION
Citations
2244 | WordNet: A Lexical Database for English
- Miller
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... size 24 Helvetica font, in the center of the picture stimuli.sThese parameters wereschosen to make the string highly visible.sRelated distractor words were selected fromsthe online WordNet database (=-=Miller, 1995-=-; WordNet, 2010), to be a non-cognatessister term of the target picture item–that is, sharing a hypernym (e.g., “pet” for “cat”sand “dog”) with the target item (Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Schriefers et... |
1561 |
Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions
- Stroop
- 1935
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...WI task has been shown to require inhibitory mechanisms thatsoverlap with those used during the Stroop task, (see de Zubricaray, Wilson,sMcMahon & Muthiah, 2001), a classic cognitive control measure (=-=Stroop, 1935-=-) andsso interactions of language switching with the PWI task may yet reflect sharedsreliance on more domain general cognitive control mechanisms, beyond the lexicalslevel.sIn addition, this paradigm ... |
1141 |
Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models. Cambridge University Press, 1st edition
- Gelman, Hill
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...t RT’s.s21smodels with random-effect slopes, in part due to difficulty calculating degrees ofsfreedom, t-values with an absolute value greater than 2 were considered to indicate assignificant effect (=-=Gelman & Hill, 2007-=-).sAccuracy.sFor all experiments, accuracy data from the main tasks aresreported both for descriptive statistics and statistical analyses along with the RT datasfor experiment.sDue to the nature of th... |
877 | An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part I. An account of basic findings - McClelland, Rumelhart - 1981 |
742 | Conflict monitoring and cognitive control
- Botvinick, Braver, et al.
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ality is not usually discussedsin conflict adaptation literature.sThat is, though typically conflict adaptation issthought of as adapting to conflict, in that conflict reduces subsequent costs (e.g.,s=-=Botvinick et al., 2001-=-), this might not be the case.sIn fact, the “Gratton effect”s(Gratton et al., 1992), upon which conflict adaptation is based, is framed as findingsincreased interference following congruent trials; an... |
741 | A theory of lexical access in speech production - Levelt, Roelofs, et al. - 1999 |
560 | Psyscope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments - Cohen, MacWhittney, et al. - 1993 |
508 |
Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks.
- Rogers, Monsell
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...g the Mix program (van Casteren & Davis, 2006) according to the followingsconstraints:s1. A fixed order for L1 and L2 language cues with a pattern of L1, L1,sL2, L2, based on alternating runs design (=-=Rogers & Monsell, 1995-=-).s17s2. A minimum distance of 10 items between two of the same picturesitems.s3. A maximum of 5 sequential trials with the same arrow congruencyscondition.s4. A maximum of 4 sequential trials with th... |
432 | Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory
- Pashler
- 1994
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...asse at as‘central bottleneck’, which requires the first task be completed before the second.sItsis argued that when a task is subject to this bottleneck, it indicates use of domaingeneral resources (=-=Pashler, 1994-=-).sNote, that not all tasks are subject to thissbottleneck; for example automatized processes such as word reading may bypass thesbottleneck, and it is important to consider how this may effect the re... |
375 | lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (R package version 1.1-7)” [Statistical software], - Bates, Maechler, et al. - 2014 |
285 |
Task switching
- Monsell
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...e evidence that there is ascost associated with a switch between tasks (measured in increased reaction time in asswitch trial vs. stay trial; cf. switching costs outside of the linguistic domain; sees=-=Monsell, 2003-=-, for a review).sThere are often findings that switching costs are,scounter-intuitively, typically smaller when switching from a stronger to a weaker tasks(Wylie & Allport, 2000; cf. Monsell, Yeung & ... |
217 | Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control - Kerns, Cohen, et al. - 2004 |
212 |
Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses
- Gratton, Coles, et al.
- 1992
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...pose that in the context of congruentsand incongruent choices, a congruent trial encourages greater bottom-up (parallel)sprocessing compared to top-down (focused) processing of the upcoming stimuluss(=-=Gratton, et al., 1992-=-).sThis finding, termed conflict adaptation (or sometimes thesGratton effect) is more typically discussed as a reduced interference effect (that is, thes1 The flanker task is an inhibitory control tas... |
209 | The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components
- Indefrey, Levelt
- 2004
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...onset range was chosen so thatsthe arrow would likely appear during lemma selection, the point of predictedscompetition in the production process, based on estimates of the timing of wordsproduction (=-=Indefrey & Levelt, 2004-=-).sThus, the conflict from the arrow was timed tostax inhibitory resources at the predicted time point of lexical conflict in the languagesswitching task.sThe picture remained on the screen until the ... |
190 | How Many Levels of Processing are There in Lexical Access?
- Caramazza
- 1997
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... this chosen lexical item.sThe process however, is morescomplex than a single mapping from concept to sound, and it is generally agreed thatsit involves a hierarchy of stages from start to end (e.g., =-=Caramazza, 1997-=-; Dell &sO’Seaghdha, 1992; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999).sThough the procedure ofsspeaking involves processes at multiple stages, the focus of this paper specificallysconcerns processing involved in... |
177 |
Mental Control of the Bilingual Lexico-Semantic System
- Green
- 1998
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ith this conflict is still unsettled, however,san influential model of bilingual production, the inhibitory model, proposes thatsbilinguals use inhibitory control to suppress the non-target language (=-=Green, 1998-=-).sSpecifically, the claim is tied to the role of domain-general control, suggesting that,srather than a mechanism devoted specifically to language control, these mechanismssare likely to also be invo... |
175 |
Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture–word interference studies.
- Schriefers, Meyer, et al.
- 1990
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ibed in the introduction, PWIstasks require naming pictures while ignoring a distractor word, which adds conflict inslexical selection when the distractor is semantically similar to the target (e.g.,s=-=Schriefers et al., 1990-=-; Meyer & Schriefers, 1991).sAs such, this paradigm allowedsfor manipulation of level of conflict on a naming trial by manipulating the distractorsword’s relation to the picture.sThis paradigm also el... |
173 | The analysis of sentence production - Garrett - 1975 |
109 | Auditory S–R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing - Simon, Rudell - 1967 |
107 | The time course of picture-word interference. - Glaser, Dungelhoff - 1984 |
93 | Stages of lexical access in language production. - Dell, O’Seaghdha - 1992 |
92 |
The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: Indirect election of words.
- Kempen, Huijbers
- 1983
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... consensus model of word production, which focuses on these stages, issdepicted below in Figure 1a.sAs shown, a lexical concept activates an array of lexicalsitems.sThese items, (often called lemmas; =-=Kempen & Huijbers, 1983-=-), are abstractsword representations which carry semantic and syntactic information without wordform information (thus, for instance, distinguish between homonyms; Kempen &sHuijbers, 1983; Levelt et a... |
89 | Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind.
- Bialystok
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...ibitory control in guiding her language use may transfer to improvedsperformance in domain-general attention and inhibitory control tasks, i.e., thesbilingual advantage (e.g., Abutalebi et al., 2012, =-=Bialystok, 1999-=-; Bialystok andsMartin, 2004; Bialystok, Craik, Grandy, et al., 2005; but see de Bruin, Treccani, &sDella Sala, 2014; Papp & Greenberg, 2013).s6sExperimental support for the general role of inhibitory... |
85 |
Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection
- Meuter, Allport
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...alancedsbilinguals, an analogous switch cost asymmetry.sThat is, there is a larger cost whensswitching into a dominant language (L1) than into a less dominant L2 (e.g., Guo, Liu,sMisra & Kroll, 2011; =-=Meuter & Allport, 1999-=-, but see Finkbeiner et al, 2006, Verhoefset al., 2009).sThese findings generally suggest that the L1 is a stronger competitor forsselection than the L2, and thus needs to be suppressed to a greater e... |
72 | Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. - Bialystok, Martin - 2004 |
68 | Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. - Abutalebi, Green - 2007 |
67 |
Lexical selection in bilinguals: Do words in the bilingual's two lexicons compete for selection?.
- Costa, Miozzo, et al.
- 1999
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nsgreater.sIt is generally agreed, and is well substantiated with evidence of crossslanguage facilitation and interference, that multiple languages are in fact active at thespoint of selection (e.g., =-=Costa, Miozzo, & Caramazza, 1999-=-, Kroll, Bobb &sWodniecka, 2006), and thus that there is additional competition in bilingualsproduction (e.g., Bialystok & Craik, 2010 Gollan & Silverberg, 2001).sFigure 1bsdepicts a schematic of bili... |
67 | Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task - Monsell, Yeung, et al. - 2000 |
66 | Dissociations and dependencies between speed and accuracy: evidence for a two-component theory of divided attention in simple tasks.
- Pashler
- 1989
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...dness Pi ctu resN am ingsT im e switch_cond stay switchs56sThe under additive interaction could be could be explained, in part, in terms ofsa bottleneck in processing (e.g., Ferreira & Pashler, 2002; =-=Pashler, 1989-=-) wherebyssome, but not all aspects of a given task require domain general, capacity-limitedsresources.sWhen capacity-demanding tasks are presented simultaneously, they reachsa bottleneck, and must be... |
62 | Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words. - Damian, Vigliocco, et al. - 2001 |
54 | Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli
- Meyer, Schriefers
- 1991
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... PWIstasks require naming pictures while ignoring a distractor word, which adds conflict inslexical selection when the distractor is semantically similar to the target (e.g.,sSchriefers et al., 1990; =-=Meyer & Schriefers, 1991-=-).sAs such, this paradigm allowedsfor manipulation of level of conflict on a naming trial by manipulating the distractorsword’s relation to the picture.sThis paradigm also eliminated any direct change... |
53 | Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. - Costa, Santesteban - 2004 |
50 |
The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals
- Marian, Blumenfeld, et al.
- 2007
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...matic of Experiment 2.sSquare and circles surrounding the digit arescues to name the digit in the L1, and L2, respectively.sQuestionnaire.sThe participants completed a shortened version of the LEAPQ (=-=Marian et al, 2007-=-), after completing the switching task.sThese questions includedsnumber of languages spoken, order of acquisition and dominance, and self-ratedscomfort in speaking, reading, and writing the two langua... |
42 | On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. - Costa, Hernandez, et al. - 2009 |
42 |
The flanker task and response competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems.
- Eriksen
- 1995
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...task can in turn reduce conflict on an upcoming task, positing ansunder additive interaction between the two tasks.sGratton, Coles and Donchin (1992)sdemonstrated this with a continuous flanker task (=-=Eriksen, 1995-=-)1 used to investigatesadjustments in response after conflict, vs. non-conflict trials.sInterestingly, theresseemed to be more susceptibility to interference on trials that followed congruent,scompare... |
41 | Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production. - Bloem, Heij, et al. - 2003 |
39 | When more is less: A counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture–word interference paradigm.
- Miozzo, Caramazza
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...wherein low frequency distractors, believedsto have lower levels of activation in the mental lexicon (e.g., McClelland &sRumelhart, 1981), may cause more interference than high frequency distractorss(=-=Miozzo & Caramazza, 2003-=-).sYet, while the details of lexical interference in wordsproduction are clearly complicated and still under debate, it is nevertheless clear thatscompetition in selection does exist and plays a role ... |
37 | Central bottleneck influences on the processing stages of word production.
- Ferreira, Pashler
- 2002
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... Related Distractor relatedness Pi ctu resN am ingsT im e switch_cond stay switchs56sThe under additive interaction could be could be explained, in part, in terms ofsa bottleneck in processing (e.g., =-=Ferreira & Pashler, 2002-=-; Pashler, 1989) wherebyssome, but not all aspects of a given task require domain general, capacity-limitedsresources.sWhen capacity-demanding tasks are presented simultaneously, they reachsa bottlene... |
37 | Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. - Kroll, Bobb, et al. - 2006 |
35 | Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG. - Bialystok, Craik, et al. - 2005 |
33 | Control and interference in task switching—a review.
- Kiesel, Steinhauser, et al.
- 2010
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...e complete story.sOther research has suggested that interference in switching tasks is not fromsinhibition of the dominant task, but is instead from activation of a non-dominant tasks(for review, see =-=Kiesel et al., 2010-=-).sAdditionally, there is some dispute about thesinterpretation of a switch cost asymmetry as a marker for inhibitory control (Bobbsand Wodnieka, 2013).sAs such, task maintenance and activation may al... |
24 |
Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load.
- Lavie
- 2010
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...that is responsible for their interaction.sPerceptual load, such increased visualscomplexity, has been shown to produce very different effects on attentional processessthan a working memory load (see =-=Lavie, 2010-=- for a review).sSpecifically, evidencesfrom perceptual load studies show that increasing task difficulty by, for examplesincreasing visual complexity in a scene, may actually reduce interference andsi... |
21 | On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture–word interference paradigm - Costa, Alario, et al. - 2005 |
20 | Lexical selection in bilingual speech production does not involve language suppression.
- Finkbeiner, Almeida, et al.
- 2006
(Show Context)
Citation Context ... switch cost asymmetry.sThat is, there is a larger cost whensswitching into a dominant language (L1) than into a less dominant L2 (e.g., Guo, Liu,sMisra & Kroll, 2011; Meuter & Allport, 1999, but see =-=Finkbeiner et al, 2006-=-, Verhoefset al., 2009).sThese findings generally suggest that the L1 is a stronger competitor forsselection than the L2, and thus needs to be suppressed to a greater extent in order tosproduce the L2... |
18 |
Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, Epub ahead of print retrieved December 12, 2011, from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/10/25/cercor.bhr287
- Abutalebi, D, et al.
- 2011
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...fetime ofspracticing inhibitory control in guiding her language use may transfer to improvedsperformance in domain-general attention and inhibitory control tasks, i.e., thesbilingual advantage (e.g., =-=Abutalebi et al., 2012-=-, Bialystok, 1999; Bialystok andsMartin, 2004; Bialystok, Craik, Grandy, et al., 2005; but see de Bruin, Treccani, &sDella Sala, 2014; Papp & Greenberg, 2013).s6sExperimental support for the general r... |
18 | Broca’s area and language processing: Evidence for the cognitive control connection. - Novick, Trueswell, et al. - 2010 |
16 |
There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing.
- Paap, Greenberg
- 2013
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...asks, i.e., thesbilingual advantage (e.g., Abutalebi et al., 2012, Bialystok, 1999; Bialystok andsMartin, 2004; Bialystok, Craik, Grandy, et al., 2005; but see de Bruin, Treccani, &sDella Sala, 2014; =-=Papp & Greenberg, 2013-=-).s6sExperimental support for the general role of inhibitory control in bilingualsproduction is frequently shown in the context of language-switching tasks.sSimilar tosclassic task-switching paradigms... |
13 | The role of inhibition in task switching: A review - Koch, Gade, et al. - 2010 |
13 | Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. - Verhoef, Roelofs, et al. - 2009 |
11 | Set size and repetition in the pictureword interference paradigm: implications for models of naming - Caramazza, Costa - 2001 |
11 |
Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks
- Philipp, Koch
- 2009
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...rements of the upcoming task, rather than simply an effectsof prior-task inhibition, reconfiguration does not appear to account for the cost in itssentirety (e.g., Koch, Gade, Schuch & Philipp, 2010; =-=Philipp & Koch, 2009-=-; Wylie &sAllport, 2000).sSpecifically, evidence that language switching costs reflect inhibitionsof the non-target language and not just task set reconfiguration comes from Philippsand Koch (2009), w... |
10 |
Linking inhibition to activation in the control of task sequences
- Gade, Koch
- 2005
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...scussion.sOther accounts that offer alternatives to the inhibitory control hypothesis aresalso based on activation, rather than inhibition.sFirst, “persistent activation” (e.g.,sPhilipp et al., 2007; =-=Gade & Koch, 2005-=-) suggests that strong activation of thesweaker task persists, making the upcoming switch into a dominant task more difficult.sThus, rather than dealing with overcoming previous inhibition, persistent... |
10 |
Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: language and cognition
- Gollan, Silverberg
- 2001
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...at thespoint of selection (e.g., Costa, Miozzo, & Caramazza, 1999, Kroll, Bobb &sWodniecka, 2006), and thus that there is additional competition in bilingualsproduction (e.g., Bialystok & Craik, 2010 =-=Gollan & Silverberg, 2001-=-).sFigure 1bsdepicts a schematic of bilingual production, demonstrating increased competitionsfrom the additional active lemmas.s4sFigure 1.sa)s(Left): Model of monolingual speech production process (... |
10 | Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. - Linck, Schwieter, et al. - 2012 |
9 | Constraints upon word substitution speech errors. - Harley, MacAndrew - 2001 |
7 |
Resolving semantic interference during word production requires central attention
- Kleinman
- 2013
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...sks are subject to thissbottleneck; for example automatized processes such as word reading may bypass thesbottleneck, and it is important to consider how this may effect the relative timing ofstasks (=-=Kleinman, 2013-=-).sAnother way to interfere with lexical selection is bysmanipulating demands on working memory.sBelke (2013) demonstrated this using asblocked cyclic naming task, comparing item naming in homogenous ... |
7 | How decisions evolve: The temporal dynamics of action selection. - Scherbaum, Dshemuchadse, et al. - 2010 |
5 | Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning - Bobb, Wodniecka - 2013 |
5 | Challenging a decade of brain research on task switching: brain activation in the task-switching paradigm reflects adaptation rather than reconfiguration of task sets - Baene, Kühn, et al. - 2012 |
5 | Cognitive advantage in bilingualism An example of publication bias - Bruin, Treccani, et al. - 2015 |
5 | Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. - Guo, Liu, et al. - 2011 |
3 |
To adapt or not to adapt: The question of domain-general cognitive control
- Kan, Teubner-Rhodes, et al.
- 2013
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...nt trial allows more efficient responding to a subsequentsincongruent stimulus.sAs it has been shown that conflict adaptation can occursbetween tasks that share similar levels of conflict processing (=-=Kan et al., 2013-=-), it isspossible that adaptation could potentially occur between the two tasks in a dual tasksparadigm.sIf so, an under-additive effect might indicate a shared level of conflictsprocessing between th... |
3 | Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing - Mercier, Pivneva, et al. - 2013 |
2 | Is “conflict adaptation” driven by conflict? Behavioral and EEG evidence for the underappreciated role of congruent trials - Compton, Huber, et al. - 2012 |
2 | A new on-online resource for psycholinguistic studies - Szekeley, Jacobsen, et al. - 2012 |
2 | Mix, a program for pseudorandomization. Behavior Research methods - Casteren, Davis - 2006 |
1 | Bilingualism Language and Cognition Language and Cognition: Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish – English and English – Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and - Costa, Caramazza - 2000 |
1 |
Locus of semantic interference in picture– word interference tasks
- Damian, Bowers
- 2003
(Show Context)
Citation Context ...stractor relationship.sClassically, this task has found slowed picture naming when distractor wordss(presented either aurally or superimposed in print), are semantically related to thespictured item (=-=Damian & Bowers, 2003-=-; Glaser, & Düngelhoff, 1984; Schriefers,sMeyer & Levelt, 1990).sAs the production model suggests, a lexical concept willsactivate not only its lemma but also semantically similar lemmas.sThus, assema... |