• Documents
  • Authors
  • Tables
  • Log in
  • Sign up
  • MetaCart
  • DMCA
  • Donate

CiteSeerX logo

Advanced Search Include Citations
Advanced Search Include Citations

A typology of consonant agreement as correspondence. (2004)

by S Rose, R Walker
Venue:Lg
Add To MetaCart

Tools

Sorted by:
Results 1 - 10 of 131
Next 10 →

A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning

by Bruce Hayes, Colin Wilson , 2006
"... The study of phonotactics (e.g., the ability of English speakers to distinguish possible words like blick from impossible words like *bnick) is a central topic in phonology. We propose a theory of phonotactic grammars and a learning algorithm that constructs such grammars from positive evidence. Our ..."
Abstract - Cited by 136 (15 self) - Add to MetaCart
The study of phonotactics (e.g., the ability of English speakers to distinguish possible words like blick from impossible words like *bnick) is a central topic in phonology. We propose a theory of phonotactic grammars and a learning algorithm that constructs such grammars from positive evidence. Our grammars consist of constraints that are assigned numerical weights according to the principle of maximum entropy. Possible words are assessed by these grammars based on the weighted sum of their constraint violations. The learning algorithm yields grammars that can capture both categorical and gradient phonotactic patterns. The algorithm is not provided with any constraints in advance, but uses its own resources to form constraints and weight them. A baseline model, in which Universal Grammar is reduced to a feature set and an SPE-style constraint format, suffices to learn many phonotactic phenomena. In order to learn nonlocal phenomena such as stress and vowel harmony, it is necessary to augment the model with autosegmental tiers and metrical grids. Our results thus offer novel, learning-theoretic support for such representations. We apply the model to English syllable onsets, Shona vowel harmony, quantity-insensitive stress typology, and the full phonotactics of Wargamay, showing that the learned grammars capture the distributional generalizations of these languages and accurately predict the findings of a phonotactic experiment.

Analytic bias and phonological typology

by Elliott Moreton , 2008
"... Two factors have been proposed as the main determinants of phonological typology: channel bias, phonetically systematic errors in transmission, and analytic bias, cognitive predispositions making learners more receptive to some patterns than others. Much of typology can be explained equally well by ..."
Abstract - Cited by 33 (4 self) - Add to MetaCart
Two factors have been proposed as the main determinants of phonological typology: channel bias, phonetically systematic errors in transmission, and analytic bias, cognitive predispositions making learners more receptive to some patterns than others. Much of typology can be explained equally well by either factor, making them hard to distinguish empirically. This study presents evidence that analytic bias is strong enough to create typological asymmetries in a case where channel bias is controlled. I show that (i) phonological dependencies between the height of two vowels are typologically more common than dependencies between vowel height and consonant voicing, (ii) the phonetic precursors of the heightheight and height-voice patterns are equally robust and (iii) in two experiments, English speakers learned a height-height pattern and a voice-voice pattern better than a height-voice pattern. I conclude that both factors contribute to typology, and discuss hypotheses about their interaction.

Inductive Learning of Phonotactic Patterns

by Jeffrey Nicholas Heinz , 2007
"... ..."
Abstract - Cited by 24 (8 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract not found

Stochastic phonological knowledge: The case of Hungarian vowel harmony

by Bruce Hayes, Zsuzsa Cziráky Londe - PHONOLOGY , 2006
"... In Hungarian, stems ending in a back vowel plus one or more neutral vowels show unusual behavior: for such stems, the otherwise-general process of vowel harmony is lexically idiosyncratic. Particular stems can take front suffixes, take back suffixes, or vacillate. Yet at a statistical level, the p ..."
Abstract - Cited by 22 (0 self) - Add to MetaCart
In Hungarian, stems ending in a back vowel plus one or more neutral vowels show unusual behavior: for such stems, the otherwise-general process of vowel harmony is lexically idiosyncratic. Particular stems can take front suffixes, take back suffixes, or vacillate. Yet at a statistical level, the patterning among these stems is lawful: in the aggregate, they obey principles that relate the propensity to take back or front harmony to the height of the rightmost vowel and to the number of neutral vowels. We argue that this patterned statistical variation in the Hungarian lexicon is internalized by native speakers. Our evidence is that they replicate the pattern when they are asked to apply harmony to novel stems in a “wug ” test (Berko 1958). Our test results match quantitative data about the Hungarian lexicon, gathered with an automated Web search. We model the speakers’ knowledge and intuitions with a grammar based on the dual listing/generation model of Zuraw (2000), then show how the constraint rankings of this grammar can be learned by algorithm.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...rigger wins” principle was the automatic consequence of the ban on crossed association lines (Clements 1977). However, in light of recent research (Hansson 2001; Frisch, Pierrehumbert, and Broe 2004; =-=Rose and Walker 2004-=-) it appears that the “closest trigger wins” principle is not exceptionless; and it is entirely incompatible with the view taken here, in which variation in Hungarian is attributed to conflicting harm...

For an autosegmental theory of mutation

by Matthew Wolf - UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST , 2007
"... ..."
Abstract - Cited by 20 (5 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract not found

Headed spans and autosegmental spreading

by John J. Mccarthy , 2004
"... ..."
Abstract - Cited by 19 (1 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract not found

On languages piecewise testable in the strict sense

by James Rogers, Jeffrey Heinz, Gil Bailey, Matt Edlefsen, Molly Visscher, David Wellcome, Sean Wibel - In Proceedings of the 11th Meeting of the Assocation for Mathematics of Language , 2009
"... Abstract. In this paper we explore the class of Strictly Piecewise languages, originally introduced to characterize long-distance phonotactic patterns by Heinz [1] as the Precedence Languages. We provide a series of equivalent abstract characterizations, discuss their basic properties, locate them r ..."
Abstract - Cited by 18 (12 self) - Add to MetaCart
Abstract. In this paper we explore the class of Strictly Piecewise languages, originally introduced to characterize long-distance phonotactic patterns by Heinz [1] as the Precedence Languages. We provide a series of equivalent abstract characterizations, discuss their basic properties, locate them relative to other well-known subregular classes and provide algorithms for translating between the grammars defined here and finite state automata as well as an algorithm for deciding whether a regular language is Strictly Piecewise. 1
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...xample in (1c). ⋆ The author acknowledges the support of a 2008-2009 University of Delaware Research Fund Grant. 3 Linguistic descriptions of Sarcee (and many other languages with consonantal harmony =-=[9,10]-=-) are clear that agreeing consonants can be arbitrarily distant.1. a. /si-tSiz-aP/ → SítSídzàP ‘my duck’ c. cf. *sítSídzàP b. /na-s-GatS/ → nāSGátS ‘I killed them again’ Heinz [1, 6] observes that th...

Explaining final obstruent voicing in Lezgian: phonetics and history

by Alan C. L. Yu - Language , 2004
"... In Lezgian, a Nakh-Daghestanian language, final and preconsonantal ejectives and voiceless unaspirated obstruents are voiced in certain monosyllabic nouns. This article offers acoustic evi-dence confirming that the two coda-voicing series are indeed voiced in final position. Based on comparative evi ..."
Abstract - Cited by 17 (2 self) - Add to MetaCart
In Lezgian, a Nakh-Daghestanian language, final and preconsonantal ejectives and voiceless unaspirated obstruents are voiced in certain monosyllabic nouns. This article offers acoustic evi-dence confirming that the two coda-voicing series are indeed voiced in final position. Based on comparative evidence, it is demonstrated that this phonetically aberrant neutralization pattern is the result of a series of phonetically natural sound changes. Such ‘crazy rules ’ (Bach & Harms 1972) undermine any direct phonetic licensing approach to phonology, such as LICENSING BY CUE (Steriade 1997).*

Linguistic optimization

by Joe Pater, Rajesh Bhatt, Christopher Potts
"... Optimality Theory (OT) is a model of language that combines aspects of generative and connectionist linguistics. It is unique in the field in its use of a rank ordering on constraints, which is used to formalize optimization, the choice of the best of a set of potential linguistic forms. We show tha ..."
Abstract - Cited by 16 (2 self) - Add to MetaCart
Optimality Theory (OT) is a model of language that combines aspects of generative and connectionist linguistics. It is unique in the field in its use of a rank ordering on constraints, which is used to formalize optimization, the choice of the best of a set of potential linguistic forms. We show that phenomena argued to require ranking fall out equally from the form of optimization in OT’s predecessor Harmonic Grammar (HG), which uses numerical weights to encode the relative strength of constraints. We further argue that the known problems for HG can be resolved by adopting assumptions about the nature of constraints that have precedents both in OT and elsewhere in computational and generative linguistics. This leads to a formal proof that if the range of each constraint is a bounded number of violations, HG generates a finite number of languages. This is nontrivial, since the set of possible weights for each constraint is nondenumerably infinite. We also briefly review some advantages of HG. 1
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...g possible from impossible languages. Challenges for a theory with local evaluation are apparent non-local interactions, such as the ban on multiple voiced obstruents in Japanese (see fn. 4; see also =-=Rose and Walker 2004-=- for discussion of long-distance assimilation). It also remains to be better determined how HG and OT fare respectively under local evaluation. As far as we know, the only published example of overgen...

Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning, Part II: Substance

by Elliott Moreton, Joe Pater , 2012
"... Artificial analogues of natural-language phonological patterns can often be learned in the lab from small amounts of training or exposure. The difficulty of a featurallydefined pattern has been hypothesized to be affected by two main factors, its formal structure (the abstract logical relationships ..."
Abstract - Cited by 13 (3 self) - Add to MetaCart
Artificial analogues of natural-language phonological patterns can often be learned in the lab from small amounts of training or exposure. The difficulty of a featurallydefined pattern has been hypothesized to be affected by two main factors, its formal structure (the abstract logical relationships between the defining features) and its phonetic substance (the concrete phonetic interpretation of the pattern). This paper, the second of a two-part series, reviews the experimental literature on phonetic substance, which is hypothesized to facilitate the acquisition of phonological patterns that resemble naturally-occurring phonetic patterns. The effects of phonetic substance on pattern learning turn out to be elusive and unreliable in comparison with the robust effects of formal complexity (reviewed in Part I). If natural-language acquisition is guided by the same inductive biases as are found in the lab, these results support a theory in which inductive bias shapes only the form, and channel bias shapes the content, of the sound patterns of the worlds languages.
(Show Context)

Citation Context

...iggered by following rather than preceding vocoids, and by high rather than low vocoids (Kim, 2001), and vowel-height harmony is more common than consonant continuancy harmony (Hansson 2001, 137–149, =-=Rose and Walker 2004-=-). Very often, the more-frequent phonological pattern is a stylized version of some kind of phonetic covariation (e.g., phonological vowel harmony resembles phonetic vowel-to-vowel coarticulation). On...

Powered by: Apache Solr
  • About CiteSeerX
  • Submit and Index Documents
  • Privacy Policy
  • Help
  • Data
  • Source
  • Contact Us

Developed at and hosted by The College of Information Sciences and Technology

© 2007-2019 The Pennsylvania State University